Boys, Girls, and the Pursuit of Science

This guest post was written by Tracyanne Roberts, the Education and Public Outreach (EPO) Coordinator for CalTech/IPAC. Tracy is the lead on a NASA Universe of Learning project called STEMdex, a project to streamline access for the astronomy EPO community and improve knowledge of published STEM (Science, Technology, Engineering, and Math) education research literature.

Title 1: Gender stereotypes about intellectual ability emerge early and influence children’s interests
Authors: Lin Bian, Sarah-Jane Leslie, Andrei Cimpian
First Author’s Institution: University of Illinois, New York University
Status: Published in Science, January 2017

Title 2: Attitudes Towards Science: An Update
Authors: Jonathan Osborne, Shirley Simon, Russell Tytler
First Author’s Institution: Stanford University
Status: Presented at Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, April 2017


Introduction

How smart are you? Did you get where you are today by sheer brilliance, or due to copious hard work? When did you become a science geek? And hey — are you a nice person? Perhaps not a surprise — but your answer to these questions likely depends upon your gender. Even more distressing, it is likely you formed views of your intelligence before you finished kindergarten! And furthermore, whether or not you became a scientist was likely set before you finished middle school.

Students’ interest in science may already be determined by age 14, according to a new study. [Brendan Fitterer | Tampa Bay Times]

Such are the results from two recently presented papers that examine student attitudes about brilliance and scientific interest. The first paper points to the startling supposition that, at the grand old age of 6, girls begin to view their own gender as less intelligent than males and begin to alter their activities and interests accordingly. The second paper strongly points to students’ interest in science being set between the ages of 10 and 14. These papers jointly support the notion that very young children begin to alter their interests and activities based upon stereotypes, and that by age 14, individuals have largely developed toward becoming a science enthusiast … or not.

Common stereotypes associate high-level intellectual ability (brilliance, genius, etc.) with men more so than with women (and it seems that most people associate being nice with being female, by the way). A new study has found that by age 6, girls are less likely than boys to believe that females are “really, really smart.” Also at that time, girls begin to avoid an activity if told the endeavor is for smart people. “Gender Stereotypes About Intellectual Ability Emerge Early and Influence Children’s Interests” suggests that children acquire stereotypes about brilliance during first grade and that immediately begins to affect their interests. Tragically, the slight differences in daily choices about games and activities can accumulate over years, leading to life-changing gender gaps in experience and knowledge. The glass ceiling begins in elementary school.

“Even though the stereotype equating brilliance with men doesn’t match reality, it might nonetheless take a toll on girls’ aspirations and on their eventual careers,” observed Cimpian, the paper’s senior author.

In agreement with Cimpian’s statement, the paper “Attitudes Towards Science: An Update” finds that “…by the age of 14, for the majority of students, interest or not in pursuing further study of science has largely been formed. Moreover, in the case of girls, their attitude is significantly more negative, particularly toward the physical sciences.”


Paper 1: Boys Are Smart, Girls Are Nice, but Everyone Can Try Hard

The studies in “Gender Stereotypes About Intellectual Ability Emerge Early and Influence Children’s Interests” were undertaken to examine the age at which brilliance stereotypes begin to arise. In one task, the children were told a brief story about a person who was “really, really smart.” Children then viewed pictures of four unfamiliar “targets” (two male, two female), and were asked to identify the protagonist of the story.

stereotype scores

Figure 1: Boys’ (blue) and girls’ (red) stereotype scores in two independent studies. A and C show how children of different ages rated the brilliance of their own gender. B and D show how they rated their gender’s niceness.  [Bian et al. 2017]

All of the “targets” were deemed to be of similar attractiveness and dress to avoid potential subtle bias. In some studies, all targets were adults whereas in other studies, the targets were pictures of adults and/or other children. The results did not differ significantly by whether children rated adult versus child targets, and were similar across various socioeconomic and racial-ethnic backgrounds within the study.

In other tasks, the children saw several pairs of same- or different-gender targets and guessed which person was “really, really smart” or “really, really nice.” Common stereotypes of being “smart” and of being “nice” are traits that are frequently differentially linked to gender, and could provide further information about the age at which stereotypes develop.

The results showed that both boys and girls aged 5 viewed their own gender as intelligent. But girls aged 6 and 7 were significantly less likely than boys to associate brilliance with their gender (Figures 1A, 1C). The stereotype associating females as being nice follows a similar development (Figures 1B, 1D). In both studies, the results suggest that children’s ideas about “brilliance” and “niceness” exhibit rapid changes from age 5 to 7. Thus, the stereotypes that “brilliance = males” and “niceness = females” may already be embraced by children as young as age 6.

responses to games

Figure 2: Boys’ (blue) and girls’ (red) interest (average of standardized responses to four questions) in two novel games. A comparison of responses to the Smart Game vs. the Try-Hard Game is on the left, and a comparison of different age responses to the Smart Game is on the right. [Bian et al. 2017]

Next, the authors investigated whether children’s gendered beliefs influence their activities and interests. In the next studies, children were introduced to two games, one said to be for “children who are really, really smart” and the other for “children who try really, really hard.” The content and rules of both games were very similar. Children were asked four questions to measure their interest in these games (e.g., “Do you like this game, or do you not like it?”).

Girls at 5 years of age showed interest in playing the smart game, but at age six, exhibited a markedly decreased interest (Figure 2B). In contrast, both genders exhibited similar interests in the try-hard game, with girls expressing a slightly greater interest. Combined with additional tasks, the results indicate that young children’s emerging notions about intelligence guides their decision regarding which activities they want to participate in.

Indeed, if these attitudes alter a child’s activities during elementary school, and science is stereotypically considered “for really, really smart people” rather than for people who “try really, really hard,” girls become increasingly unlikely to engage in activities that will lead them toward an interest in, and readiness for, a scientific career.


Paper 2: Motivation to Pursue Science Determined by Gender?

In “Attitudes Towards Science: An Update,” the authors cite a plethora of worldwide studies that provide additional convincing evidence 
that children’s 
life–world 
experiences 
prior 
to 
age 14
 are 
the 
major 
determinant 
of 
any
 decision 
to 
pursue 
the 
study 
of 
science, regardless of nationality and other social factors. Engaging older children in science becomes increasingly harder.

One especially intriguing outcome is that, among those students who are scientifically inclined, an interesting gender
 difference 
arose: females
 were more 
likely
 to
 identify with “people-related” motivators, compared
 to 
males 
whose interests are intrinsic, or “self-related.” For instance, a girl might express interest in cancer research because she knows someone who had cancer, or could “help people.” In contrast, a boy expressing interest in cancer research is more likely to be motivated by the prestige and money he would earn as a doctor. This divide is neither good nor bad, but merely provides a reflection point that a one-size-fits-all approach to educating students can never equally serve all interests. Indeed, this summary paper found many studies that strongly
 suggest
 offering 
a
 homogeneous 
curriculum
 to 
all 
is 
a
 mistake 
— 
what 
interests girls 
may not 
interest 
boys 
and
 vice 
versa. One author comments that gender “is probably 
the
 most 
significant 
variable 
related 
towards 
pupils’ 
attitude 
to
 science … In 
short, both
 the
 context, purpose
and 
implications
 matter 
for
 girls, and 
any
 attempt
 to
 present 
a
 decontextualised, value‐free
 notion 
of 
science 
will 
reduce
 their 
engagement.”

So what does motivate boys and girls to pursue science? Figure 3 reveals the scientific topics that most interest boys versus girls, according to one survey. While the stereotype of boys liking explosions may make us chuckle, it is clear from this list that we must become aware of how we interact with and motivate even the very youngest people in our spheres. Both genders are equally adept and capable, and we must become better at nurturing individual motivational differences in the youngest children to bring out the best of their abilities — whether they are science-focused or not.

Figure 3: The 5 top ranked items boys and girls would like to learn about in science. [Jenkins & Nelson 2005]

About Guest

This post was written by a guest author. If you're interested in writing a guest post for Astrobites, please contact us.

2 Comments

  1. Was this study “The 5 top ranked items boys and girls would like to learn about in science. [Jenkins & Nelson 2005]” conducted on boys and girls whose preferences had already been skewed?

    Reply
  2. I have had 15 years of experience with after school programs in Astronomy, Cooking Science, and Gardening Science. These programs have been with 8, 9, 10, and 11 year old elementary and middle school kids and special needs high school kids. I agree with a lot of the observations made by this bite but have additional perspectives I have seen first hand.

    Firstly, I am male and have been categorized as good but not necessarily nice – this is somewhat purposeful. I have had a few women categorize this as sad – now who is being stereotyped!

    I had a feminist relative who spent large amounts of time successfully stimulating my interest in science which was obvious by the time I was 6.By the time I was eleven, my nick-name in school was the “brain”, I did not accept this moniker until I was 15 as it became obvious to me that I was exceptionally smart. People then started to expect superior performance from me which exceeded my motivation – this led to failure often. Later, I finally figured out that motivation was my weakness and have spent considerable effort since motivating myself.

    In teaching young people, I find myself identifying with young people that exemplify characteristics that I see in myself and take pride in – I have purposefully become mentors to these youngsters, almost exclusively female .and not white.

    I answering young male questions about the end of the earth ( red giant phase of our sun, black holes, collision with Andromeda, etc.) I find the answers exclusively frightening to young females – their concern extends to future generations and not necessarily themselves – I find I must fully explain that 4 billion years is nothing to worry about. Size concepts, nebulae being 10 light years across and galaxies being 100,000 lights years across, also more difficult if not impossible to get across to females. I have had more difficulty with the religion vs. science conflict with females as well. I do my best in dealing with these differences with considerable success. Many times, that my success is expressed in affectionate terms by young women and I must resist their physical urges to hug me. This is not easy or fair. More like painful, to be frank.

    A few of the females that I have mentored have exhibited considerable courage venturing into advanced areas without hesitance – I find they resist the label. The ones I see exhibiting problems solving ability do not resist being identified as such. The smartest have been labeled by others so I do not use that adjective, boys or girls. I see considerable difference between science and engineering and express myself in skeptical science perspectives. Some students identify with my experience at being good at math but not necessarily liking it – they feel good about not being alone in that regard.

    I feel good about myself and how I teach – I see considerable moderation regarding this male/female stereotyping – although most is very recent. I would suggest this study must be updated regularly to reflect the rapid change I see happening. I hope I am part of that process.

    Reply

Trackbacks/Pingbacks

  1. Turning a Blind Eye: Fighting the Gender Gap in Telescope Proposal Success | astrobites - […] we’re outnumbered by men and often faced with prejudices and biases in our fields (check out these posts for…

Leave a Reply